The Evolutionary Biology of Musical Rhythm: Was Darwin Wrong?

Authors

  • Maryam Nournamaee Ph. D. Student in Linguistics, Department of Linguistics, Literature Faculty, Alzahra University, Tehran, Vanak, Iran
  • Aniruddh D. Patel Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Massachusetts, United States of America

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.22046/LA.2022.19

Keywords:

The evolution of music, Rhythm, Beat-based processing, Neural resonance, Vocal learning hypothesis, Darwin, Evolutional Biology

Abstract

In The Descent of Man, Darwin speculated that our capacity for musical rhythm reflects basic aspects of brain function broadly shared among animals. Although this remains an appealing idea, it is being challenged by modern cross-species research. This research hints that our capacity to synchronize to a beat, i.e., to move in time with a perceived pulse in a manner that is predictive and flexible across a broad range of tempi, may be shared by only a few other species. Is this really the case? If so, it would have important implications for our understanding of the evolution of human musicality.

References

Arnason U, Gullberg A, Janke A, et al. (2006). Pinniped phylogeny and a new hypothesis for their origin and dispersal. Mol Phylogenet Evol 41: 345–354.

Bregman MR, Iversen JR, Lichman D, Reinhart M, Patel AD (2012). A method for testing synchronization to a musical beat in domestic horses (Equus ferus caballus). Empir Musicol Rev 7: 144–156.

Bregman MR, Patel AD, Gentner TQ (2012). Stimulus-dependent flexibility in non-human auditory pitch processing. Cognition 122: 51–60.

Brown S, Jordania J. (2013). Universals in the world’s musics. Psychol Music 41: 229–248.

Buza´ki G (2006). Rhythms of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Chen JL, Penhune VB, Zatorre RJ (2008). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cereb Cortex 18: 2844–2854.

Chen Q, Heston JB, Burkett ZD, White SA (2013). Expression analysis of the speech-related genes FoxP1 and FoxP2 and their relation to singing behavior in two songbird species. J Exp Biol 216: 3682–3692.

Conard NJ, Malina M, Münzel SC. (2009). New flutes document the earliest musical tradition in southwestern Germany. Nature 460: 737–740.

Connor RC, Smolker R, Bejder L (2006). Synchrony, social behaviour and alliance affiliation in Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops aduncus. Anim Behav 72: 1371–1378.

Cook P, Rouse A, Wilson M, Reichmuth C (2013). A California sea lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non vocal mimic. J Comparative Psychol. 127: 412–427.

Darwin C (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.

de Schotten MT, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, Catani M (2012). Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex 48: 82–96.

Dunlap K (1910). Reactions to rhythmic stimuli, with attempt to synchronize. Psychol Rev 17: 399–416.

Eerola T, Luck G, Toiviainen P (2006). An investigation of pre-schoolers’ corporeal synchronization with music. In: Baroni M, Addessi AR, Caterina R, Costa M, editors. Proc. of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9). Bologna, Italy: ICMPC and ESCOM. pp. 472–476.

Feenders G, Liedvogel M, Rivas M, Zapka M, Horita H, et al. (2008). Molecular mapping of movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a motor theory for vocal learning origin. PLoS ONE 3: e1768. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001768

Fitch W (2013). Rhythmic cognition in humans and animals: distinguishing meter and pulse perception. Front Syst Neurosci 7: 68. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00068

Fitch WT (2006). The biology and evolution of music: a comparative perspective. Cognition 100: 173–215.

Fitch WT (2009). Biology of music: another one bites the dust. Curr Biol 19: R403–R404.

Fitch WT (2012). The biology and evolution of rhythm: unraveling a paradox. In: Rebuschat P, Rohmeier M, Hawkins JA, Cross I, editors. Language and music as cognitive systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp.73–95.

Fitch WT, Meitchen D (2013). Convergence and deep homology in the evolution of spoken language. In: Bolhuis JJ, Everaert M, editors. Birdsong, speech, and language: exploring the evolution of mind and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 45–62.

Gerhardt HC, Huber F (2002). Acoustic communication in insects and anurans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Gierhan SME (2013). Connections for auditory language in the human brain. Brain Lang 127: 205–221.

Grahn JA, Rowe JB (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. J Neurosci 29: 7540–7548.

Greenfield MD (2005). Mechanisms and evolution of communal sexual displays in arthropods and anurans. Adv Study Behav 35: 1–62.

Hagmann CE, Cook RG (2010). Testing meter, rhythm, and tempo discriminations in pigeons. Behav Processes 85: 99–110.

Hanson FE, Case JF, Buck E, Buck J (1971). Synchrony and flash entrainment in a New Guinea firefly. Science 174: 162–164.

Hasegawa A, Okanoya K, Hasegawa T, Seki Y (2011). Rhythmic synchronization tapping to an audio-visual metronome in budgerigars. Sci Rep 1: 120. doi:10.1038/srep00120

Hattori Y, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2013). Spontaneous synchronized tapping to an auditory rhythm in a chimpanzee. Sci Rep 3: 1566.

Honing H, Merchant H, Ha´den GP, Prado L, Bartolo R (2012). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) detect rhythmic groups in music, but not the beat. PLoS ONE 7:e51369.

Hove, M. J., Fairhurst, M. T., Kotz, S. A., & Keller, P. E. (2013). Synchronizing with auditory and visual rhythms: an fMRI assessment of modality differences and modality appropriateness. Neuroimage, 67, 313-321.

Hulse, S. H., & Page, S. C. (1988). Toward a comparative psychology of music perception. Music Perception, 5(4), 427-452.

Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 26(1), 59-100.

Jarvis, E. D. (2004). Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016(1), 749-777.

Jarvis ED (2013). Evolution of brain pathways for vocal learning in birds and humans. In: Bolhuis JJ, Everaert M, editors. Birdsong, speech, and language: exploring the evolution of mind and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 63–107.

Knörnschild M, Nagy M, Metz M, Mayer F, von Helversen O (2010). Complex vocal imitation during ontogeny in a bat. Biol Lett 6: 156–159.

Kung S-J, Chen JL, Zatorre RJ, Penhune VB (2013). Interacting cortical and basal ganglia networks underlying finding and tapping to the musical beat. J Cogn Neurosci 25: 401–420.

Large EW (2010). Neurodynamics of music. In: Jones M, Fay RR, Popper AN, editors. Springer handbook of auditory research, Vol. 36: music perception. New York: Springer. pp. 201–231.

Large EW, Snyder JS (2009). Pulse and meter as neural resonance. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169: 46–57.

Lewandowski B, Vyssotski A, Hahnloser RHR, Schmidt M (2013). At the interface of the auditory and vocal motor systems: NIf and its role in vocal processing, production and learning. J Physiol Paris 107: 178–192.

Lewis JW, van Essen DC (2000). Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 428: 112–137.

McDermott JH, Hauser MD (2004). Are consonant intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in a nonhuman primate. Cognition 94: B11–B21.

McDermott JH, Hauser MD (2007). Nonhuman primates prefer slow tempos but dislike music overall. Cognition 104: 654–668. 69. Brooks DI, Cook RG (2010) Chord discrimination by pigeons. Music Percept 27: 183–196.

McNeill W (1995). Keeping together in time: dance and drill in human history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Merchant H, Honing H (2013) Are non-human primates capable of rhythmic entrainment? Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis. Front Psychol 7: 274. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00274

Mirollo RE, Strogatz SH (1990) Synchronization of pulse-coupled biological oscillators. SIAM J Appl Math 50: 1645–1662.

Moore BR (1992) Avian movement imitation and a new form of mimicry: tracing the evolution of a complex form of learning. Behaviour 122: 231–263.

Moran DW, Schwartz AB (1999) Motor cortical representation of speed and direction during reaching. J Neurophysiol 82: 2676–2692.

Morley I (2013). The prehistory of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Nettl B (2000). An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and musical culture. In: Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S, editors. The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 463–472.

Nettl B, Stone R. (1998). The Garland encyclopedia of world music (10 vols). New York: Garland Publications.

Nottebohm F (1976) Phonation in the orange-winged Amazon parrot, Amazona amazonica. J Comp Physiol A 108: 157–170.

Page SC, Hulse SH, Cynx J (1989) Relative pitch perception in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris): further evidence for an elusive phenomenon. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav 15: 137–146.

Patel AD (2006) Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Percept 24: 99–104.

Patel AD (2008) Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Patel, A. D., & Demorest, S. M. (2013). 16-Comparative Music Cognition: Cross-Species and Cross-Cultural Studies. The psychology of music.

Patel, A. D., & Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Frontiers in systems neuroscience, 8, 57.

Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I (2009) Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr Biol 19: 827–830.

Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I (2009) Studying synchronization to a musical beat in nonhuman animals. Ann New York Acad Sci 1169: 459–469.

Patel AD, Iversen JR, Chen Y, Repp BH (2005) The influence of metricality and modality on synchronization with a beat. Exp Brain Res 163: 226–238.

Petkov CI, Jarvis ED (2012) Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates. Front Evol Neurosci 4: 12. doi:10.3389/fnevo.2012.00012

Rauschecker JP (2011) An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration. Hear Res 271: 16–25.

Reichmuth C, Holt MM, Muslow J, Sills JM, Southall BL (2013) Comparative assessment of amphibious hearing in pinnipeds. J Comp Physiol A 199: 491–507.

Repp BH (2005) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of the tapping literature. Psychon Bull Rev 12: 969–992.

Repp BH, Su Y-H (2013) Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bull Rev 20: 403–452.

Ridgway S, Carder D, Jeffries M, Todd M (2012) Spontaneous human speech mimicry by a cetacean. Curr Biol 22: R860–R861.

Schachner A (2010) Auditory-motor entrainment in vocal mimicking species: additional ontogenetic and phylogenetic factors. Commun Integr Biol 3: 290–293.

Schachner A (2012) If horses entrain, don’t entirely reject vocal learning: an experience-based vocal learning hypothesis. Empir Musicol Rev 7: 157–159.

Schachner A, Brady TF, Pepperberg IM, Hauser MD (2009) Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species. Curr Biol 19:831–836.

Schusterman RJ (1978) Vocal communication in pinnipeds. In: Markowitz H, Stevens VJ, editors. Behavior of captive wild animals. Chicago: Nelson Hall. pp. 247–308.

Schusterman RJ (2008) Vocal learning in mammals with special emphasis on pinnipeds. In: Oller DK, Gribel T, editors. The evolution of communicative flexibility: complexity, creativity, and adaptability in human and animal communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 41–70.

Schusterman RJ, Gentry R, Schmook J (1967) Underwater sound production by captive California sea lions, Zalophus californianus. Zoologica 52: 21–24.

Schusterman RJ, Reichmuth CJ (2008) Novel sound production via contingency learning in the Pacific walrus (Odobenus rosmarus divergens). Anim Cog 11: 319–327.

Selezneva E, Deike S, Knyazeva S, Scheich H, Brechmann A, et al. (2013) Rhythm sensitivity in macaque monkeys. Front Syst Neurosci 7:49. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00049

Snowdon CT, Teie D (2010) Affective responses in tamarins elicited by speciesspecific music. Biol Lett 6: 30–32.

Stoeger AS, Mietchen D, Oh S, de Silva S, Herbst CT, et al. (2012) An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Curr Biol 22: 2144–2148.

Tierney AT, Russo FA, Patel AD (2011) The motor origins of human and avian song structure. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 15510–15515.

Toiviainen T, Luck G, Thompson R (2010) Embodied meter: hierarchical eigenmodes in music-induced movement. Music Percept 28: 59–70.

Venneman SS (2012) A commentary on Micah Bregman et al.: a method for testing synchronization to a musical beat in domestic horses (Equus ferus caballus). Empir Musicol Rev 7: 160–163.

Winkler I, Ha´den G, Ladinig O, Sziller I, Honing H (2009) Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 2468–2471.

Wright AA, Rivera JJ, Hulse SH, Shyan M, Neiworth JJ (2000) Music perception and octave generalization in rhesus monkeys. J Exp Psychol Gen 129: 291–307.

Yin P, Fritz JB, Shamma SA (2010) Do ferrets perceive relative pitch? J Acoust Soc Am 127: 1673–1680.

Zarco w, Merchant H, Prado L, Mendez JC (2009) Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 102: 3191–3202.

Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhune VB (2007) When the brain plays music: auditorymotor interactions in music perception and production. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 547–558.

Published

2022-11-30

How to Cite

Nournamaee, M., & Patel, A. D. (2022). The Evolutionary Biology of Musical Rhythm: Was Darwin Wrong? . LANGUAGE ART, 7(4), 7–28. https://doi.org/10.22046/LA.2022.19

Similar Articles

<< < 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 > >> 

You may also start an advanced similarity search for this article.